Wednesday, June 30, 2010

DigiMedia10

Well, they've been at it again.  My students here at Fordham University, that is.  At blogging.  For this first summer session, which is almost over, I've been teaching our graduate course called Public Communication in Digital Environments (the title and course itself was created by my old colleague and fellow media ecologist, Sue Barnes).  There are only 3 students in the class, but they've been blogging up a storm, with wonderfully substantive posts, so come take a look if you're so inclined:




Thank you for your attention, and we now return you to our regularly scheduled blog, which is already in progress...

...and that's all until next time, see ya then! 

Thursday, June 24, 2010

Taken for Granted

Did my title mislead you?   No, this isn't a rant, complaint, whine, or emo-type post.  No, I just wanted to tell you about the external grant that I've been awarded by Time Warner Cable.  It's a pretty exciting project, if I do say so myself, one that draws on media ecology and general semantics as they relate to media education and media literacy, and that specifically considers how the medium of television is evolving and converging in regard to digital technologies, and the potential that those technologies hold for the future of children's media.  

And when I say that this is a grant that I've been awarded, I mean that I'm the head of the research team that also consists of my colleague at Fordham University, Lewis Freeman, and a player, er researcher, or two to be named later.

The announcement was made on May 6, 2010 (yes, I know I'm lagging behind on my blogging, my apologies).  Here's how the press release reads:

NEW RESEARCH PROGRAM AWARDS FIRST ROUND STIPENDS
 
(New York) May 6, 2010 -- Time Warner Cable (NYSE: TWC) today announced that it had awarded four stipends in its new Research Program on Digital Communications. The Research Program was launched in February 2010 to increase understanding of the benefits and challenges facing the future of digital technologies in the home, office, classroom and community. Each stipend is for $20,000. The deadline for the next round of awards is October 1, 2010. 

Fernando Laguarda, director of the new program and Vice President for External Affairs and Policy Counselor at Time Warner Cable, said, “We were very pleased with the initial research competition. We received high quality applications and believe the selected studies will add to the public debate about major issues now confronting the industry.”


And here's what our study is about:

“The Future of Children’s Television Programming: A Study of How Emerging Digital Technologies Can Facilitate Active and Engaged Participation and Contribute in Media Literacy Foundation” by Lance Strate, Professor, Department of Communication and Media Studies, Fordham University and Lewis Freeman, Professor, Department of Communication and Media Studies, Fordham University

And here's a sightly more detailed write-up:

Project Title: “The Future of Children’s Television Programming: A Study of How Emerging Digital Technologies Can Facilitate Active and Engaged Participation and Contribute in Media Literacy Foundation”
 
Researchers: Lance Strate, Professor, Department of Communication and Media Studies, Fordham University and Lewis Freeman, Professor, Department of Communication and Media Studies, Fordham University, New York, NY
 
Description: This project will increase understanding of how children can engage with television and negotiate the processes of searching, navigating, selecting and receiving the content they view. The outcome from the project will be recommendations for enhancements related to the delivery of children’s video programming using new digital technologies and new applications of current digital technologies.

I think this project is going to yield some very significant results, but of course I'm biased.  It's gratifying to be working on a project with real-world applications, which digital technologies open up in so many interesting ways.  And it's great to be able to collaborate with Lewis, who has a great deal of expertise in the area of children and media.

Special thanks to Fernando Laguarda of Time Warner Cable, I should add. And here's what it says about this new program he started:

About the Time Warner Cable Research Program on Digital Communications
 
The Time Warner Cable Research Program on Digital Communications awards stipends to foster research dedicated to increasing understanding of the benefits and challenges facing the future of digital technologies in the home, office, classroom and community. Individual researchers affiliated with universities and not-for-profits are eligible to apply for the stipends. More information about the program can be found at www.twcresearchprogram.com
And if you want to read more about the grants, and the other three projects that were awarded grants, check out the complete press release online.  As for our project, well, you'll be hearing more about it in the future, I guarantee.


Wednesday, June 23, 2010

No Remorse

So, media ecologists often point to the invention of the telegraph by Samuel F. B. Morse in 1844 as the birth of the electronic, or electric age, the origin of the electronic media environment (which is the media environment, albeit somewhat modified and evolved, that we currently inhabit).  Here's what he looked like:


By the way, in addition to being an inventor, Morse was also a renowned painter, a member of the New York University faculty (in arts and design), a proponent of slavery, an anti-Catholic and anti-immigrant activist, and a failed candidate for mayor of New York City.  Perhaps, then, it's for the best that he is not remembered as a vivid personality or well rounded historical figure in the manner of Benjamin Franklin, Thomas Edison, or even Alexander Graham Bell.

So, let's just stick with the telegraph, shall we?  That's enough to earn you a statue in Central Park, right?



Yes, the telegraph, which Morse actually developed by 1837, 1844 being its public introduction.  And actually, there were earlier inventions of the telegraph as well (see the Wikipedia entry on Telegraphy) , but let's not muddy the waters, shall we?  If nothing else, Samuel Morse did invent the Morse telegraph, after all.



And the story of why he was motivated to do so is certainly poignant.  Here's how Wikipedia tells it:

In 1825, the city of New York commissioned Morse for $1,000 to paint a portrait of Gilbert du Motier, marquis de Lafayette, in Washington. In the midst of painting, a horse messenger delivered a letter from his father that read one line, "Your dear wife is convalescent". Morse immediately left Washington for his home at New Haven, leaving the portrait of Lafayette unfinished. By the time he arrived she had already been buried. Heartbroken in the knowledge that for days he was unaware of his wife's failing health and her lonely death, he moved on from painting to pursue a means of rapid long distance communication.

The thing about the telegraph, though, is that it could transmit sounds (yes it was an acoustic medium, and the first step in reversing the visualism associated with literate and especially typographic culture, and restoring a sense of acoustic space, to use the term favored by Marshall McLuhan, Edmund Carpenter, and others), but could not produce a wide range of sound, certainly not speech.  So there needed to be a signal system or symbol system to go with the technology, a bit of software to make the hardware worth using.

And that, of course, was what became known as Morse code.  The funny thing is that Morse code wasn't actually invented by Morse, or at least, not fully.  Morse just did the numerals, and planned on having a code book with words coded by number.  It was his assistant, Alfred Vail, who came up with all of the codes for the letters of the alphabet, which is what makes the Morse code truly useful.  So I guess it should have been called Vail code, or at least Morse-Vail code, but that's the way things go here in the United States of Columbia, or is it the United States of Leifland?

Anyway, the Wikipedia entry on Morse code describes it as 

a type of character encoding that transmits telegraphic information using rhythm. Morse code uses a standardized sequence of short and long elements to represent the letters, numerals, punctuation and special characters of a given message. The short and long elements can be formed by sounds, marks, or pulses, in on off keying and are commonly known as "dots" and "dashes" or "dits" and "dahs".
On that last bit, when spoken or sometimes when written, the dot is represented by "dit" when it's by itself or appears at the end of a word, or by a "di" when it appears at the beginning of a word, or somewhere in the middle.  And the dash is represented by a "dah" for all cases.  And here's a chart, to help visualize it all:

International Morse code is a modified form of the code that was developed by Morse and Vail, but the basic principles remain the same.

So, once upon a time, telegraph and wireless telegraph operators were part of the first internet, Tom Standage referred to it as the Victorian internet, and it was basic knowledge for anyone dealing with ships and airplanes, and for amateur radio operators.  And everyone knew that S.O.S. was signaled by three dots, followed by three dashes, followed by three dots.

And no, I never learned Morse code myself, but I was a bit taken aback a few years ago when I heard that the FCC was no longer requiring knowledge of Morse code for amateur radio licenses.  It signaled the end of an era.

Does it matter?  Perhaps not, not with the internet itself providing such ease of communication, along with mobile phones and satellite phones.  It's quite possible that Morse code's time has come and gone.  But somehow it does seem to be part and parcel of the passing of an era, a lost skill not unlike reading the clockface, or a newspaper, or tying your shoes, albeit a skill that was much more specialized and limited to a minority of operators.

Anyway, it gave me an idea for a poem, albeit one that was a bit gimmicky, but it actually took me a couple of years to get past the first two lines, and figure out a way to pull it off.  I finally got it all together this past spring, and posted it over on my MySpace poetry blog earlier this month.  So, I thought I'd share it with you here on Blog Time Passing as well.


No Remorse


     There was an age
          when the wires sang
          with buzzes and beeps
               both short and long!

dah-di-dit dah-dah-dah dah di-di-dit

-••        ---         -   •••


     Back and forth the electric
          alphabet rang
          like church bells playing
               a mighty song!

dah-di-dit di-dah di-di-dit di-di-di-dit dit di-di-dit

-••        •-     •••       ••••         •   •••


     There was an age
          when the airwaves hummed
          the spectrum filled
               with unearthly tones!

dah-dah dah-dah-dah di-dah-dit di-di-dit dit

--      ---         •-•        •••       •


     Through the skies above
          the rhythms thummed
          with ethereal
               ghostly moans!

dah-di-dah-dit dah-dah-dah dah-di-dit dit

-•-•           ---         -••        •


     Operators played
          their magnetic keys
          keeping the beat
               with style and flair!

dah dit di-dah-di-dit dit

-   •   •-••          •  

dah-dah-dit di-dah-dit di-dah di-dah-dah-dit di-di-di-dit

--•         •-•        •-     •--•           ••••


     Across the continent
          and overseas
          sending their messages
               with skill and care!

di-dah-dah di-dit di-dah-dit dit

•--        ••     •-•        •

di-dah-di-dit dit di-di-dit di-di-dit

•-••          •   •••       •••


     From Maine to Texas
          and beyond
          great distances bridged
               at a speed sublime!

dit di-dit dah-dah-dit di-di-di-dit

•   ••     --•         ••••

dah dit dit dah-dit

-   •   •   -•


     Uniting us all
          in a common bond
          one neighborhood
               out of space and time!

di-di-dah-dit dah-dah-dah di-dah-dit dah dah-di-dah-dah

••-•          ---         •-•        -   -•--

di-di-dah-dit dah-dah-dah di-di-dah di-dah-dit

••-•          ---         ••-       •-•


     And now new songs
          fill the atmosphere
          and travel along wires
               that encircle the Earth!

di-di-dit di-dah dah-dah di-di-dah dit di-dah-di-dit

•••       •-     --      ••-       •   •-••


     But listening hard
          you still can hear
          the binary tune
               from that moment of birth!

dah-dah dah-dah-dah di-dah-dit di-di-dit dit

--      ---         •-•        •••       •



And here is a video illustrating the Morse code alphabet, for your listening pleasure:


And this Morse code music video is very cool indeed:


But perhaps the most dramatic use of Morse code, and one I remember seeing on television when I was a kid, was the case of Jeremiah Denton--here's an excerpt from his Wikipedia entry:
While serving as Naval Aviator during the Vietnam War, Denton was Commanding Officer (CO) of Attack Squadron SEVENTY-FIVE (VA-75) aboard the aircraft carrier USS Independence (CVA 62). On 18 July 1965, then-Commander Denton was flying an A-6A Intruder, Bureau Number 151577, from the Independence with his bombardier/navigator, Lieutenant, junior grade Bill Tschudy, to participate in a bombing mission over the Vietnamese city of Thanh Hoa. Their plane was shot down and the two men were captured by hostile forces.  Denton and Tschudy were both held as prisoners of war for almost eight years, four of which were spent in solitary confinement. Denton is best known for the 1966 North Vietnamesemorse code to spell out the word "T-O-R-T-U-R-E" to communicate that his captors were torturing him and his fellow POWs.  television interview he was forced to give as a prisoner, in which he ingeniously used the opportunity to communicate to American Intelligence. During the interview Denton blinked his eyes in
And courtesy of YouTube, here's the actual clip:


From the young David Sarnoff relaying messages about the sinking of the Titannic via wireless, to Jeremiah Denton's courageous defiance of his captors, there is something romantic and heroic about Morse code that's worthy of note, as we witness its passing into the long night of obsolescence.

And di-di-di-di-di-di-dat's all, folks!



Saturday, June 19, 2010

Scenes From MainEA


Call it MEAMaine, or MainEA, but our Eleventh Annual Media Ecology Association Convention held on June 10-13 was a great success, thanks to our gracious hosts, the University of Maine at Orono, and convention coordinators Paul Grosswiler and Ellen Rose.

Here now are some scenes from the event, and I hope you'll forgive me for mainly picking out the ones that feature me, but you may recall that I I have said on a number of occasions that this blog is mainly an exercise in narcissism.  And the rest of the pictures are available through the MEA website, if you want a more complete picture (and see also the Picassa web album for the event).

First off, here are some photographs from our program session on general semantics, as I was presenting my paper:






















































That last one wasn't too bad, I think, but you know I show these to you, warts and all.  Anyway, this particular session, which was chaired by Jackie Rudig, and also featured a paper by Geri Forsberg, and was supposed to include Corey Anton as well.  Unfortunately, Corey couldn't make it, but he was able to use Skype to present his paper from home, and that too was captured by our intrepid photographer, Octavio Islas:













See, that's Corey up on the big screen.  And you can also see a bit of the audience, and here's another view of them, taken while I was giving my talk:












And from another angle:



Of course, I was in the audience for other sessions, and here you can see me paying rapt attention:



Okay, busted, I was checked my tweets!  But I am able to multitask, you know?  Seated on my left is Geri Forsberg, by the way.   And no, I wasn't doing that constantly, look:



Anyway, later that day, I introduced Nora Bateson, daughter of Gregory Bateson, who previewed her outstanding, absolutely briliant documentary about her father, An Ecology of Mind.  I didn't find any pictures of that program, but here's a photograph of Nora taken during a break time--she's the one on the left:



And that evening, I introduced her sister, Mary Catherine Bateson (daughter of Gregory and Margaret Mead), who gave a featured presentation.  Here I am singing Mary Catherine's praises, followed by Dr. Bateson delivering her illuminating address:

The next two photos come from Facebook, courtesy of Peter Fallon, from a session that I chaired and he presented on, along with Geri Forsberg again, and Jerry Harp via Skype.  Peter is on the left, and Ed Tywoniak, rendering technical assistance, on my the right:

 

 



And here I am that evening, presenting the 2010 MEA Awards. Among the individuals that we honored were Mary Catherine Bateson, who received the MEA's Neil Postman Award for Outstanding Career Achievement in Public Intellectual Activity, and Don Ihde, recipient of the MEA's Walter Ong Award for Outstanding Career Achievement in Scholarship.

And here's a shot of our most excellent host and technomeister, Paul Grosswiler, in action:



And finally, speaking of our host, here is some ambiance, atmosphere, environment and ecology of this outstanding event:






 





 


And  after all, there is nothing like an MEA Convention, nothing in this world, at least!




Wednesday, June 16, 2010

Notes on Teaching General Semantics


In a recent post, Promoting Health, Media Ecology Style, I posted the outline notes for a presentation I made at the recent MEA meeting, and I have to confess that I am not usually in the habit of making presentations based on formal outlines, although this is the typical method taught in public speaking courses across the USA.  

As a media ecology doctoral student, Neil Postman impressed upon me that the best way to prepare a formal presentation is to write everything out, writing for the ear, but having it all in front of you.  That doesn't mean reading from the paper and never looking up, however, as he also stressed the need to practice and prepare, so that all you need to do is glance down at the paper, and then back at the audience as you deliver your address.  This is, in fact, the method that politicians and officials often use, or used before the advent of transparent screens on which speeches could be projected, so the speaker need never look down.

So, for me, when I have a formal presentation, I usually follow the Postman method.  In other instances, say on a roundtable discussion, I will just jot down some informal notes, or maybe just speak off the top of my head.  But I did use formal outlines for many years when I started out teaching classes at Fordham University and elsewhere, and I had occasion to return to some of my old notes before the MEA convention, which put the idea in my head and prompted me to create a formal outline for my presentation on health and media ecology.

The reason for my return to my old lecture notes is also connected to the MEA convention, as I was asked to prepare a handout on teaching general semantics for the event. which I did.   I don't claim that they're anything special, many others teach the same topics in the same way as I do, but the handout could possibly be helpful to someone in some way, so for that reason I was happy to share it with folks at MEA, I understand it's also going to be distributed via the Institute of General Semantics website, and I am also pleased to be able to share it with you here on my blog (and hey, if nothing else, it gives me another blog post if you know what I mean).  So, here it is:


Lance Strate’s Lecture Notes on Teaching General Semantics
Prepared for Distribution at the 11th Annual Convention of the Media Ecology Association
University of Maine, Orono, ME, June 10-13, 2010


Martin Levinson, President of the Institute of General Semantics, asked that I share my lecture notes for teaching general semantics, and I am happy to do so.  For many years, I taught a basic course introducing students to the field of communication.  These courses are known by many names, Introduction to Communication (Theory/Studies), Process of Communication, Perspectives on Communication, Concepts of Communication, etc.  My approach has been to present general semantics on its own terms, as a separate topic and unit.  I should note that my approach to teaching general semantics in this context has also been to build upon material covered in other units, and otherwise point out interconnections with topics such as perception, rhetoric, linguistics (including Sapir-Whorf and Vygotsky), semiotics and symbolic communication, metaphor, symbolic interaction, information theory and cybernetics, systems theory, and of course, media ecology, but that has varied over the years, and across different contexts, and is an option, not a requirement.  In addition to the readings listed below, these notes are drawn, in part, on graduate courses I took with Neil Postman and Christine Nystrom in the late, lamented Media Ecology Program at New York University, and also on undergraduate courses I took with Jack Barwind as an undergraduate Communication Arts major at Cornell University.  Some modifications have been made to the notes over the years, and in the course of preparing them for distribution in this form.

For those looking to familiarize themselves with general semantics, or deepen their understanding, here are some recommendations.  Wendell Johnson’s classic work, People in Quandaries, was the required text in general semantics in Neil Postman’s doctoral program in media ecology, providing a comprehensive, well organized, and clearly written discussion (albeit with some dated material on stuttering and other therapeutical issues at the end).  Postman also used Harry Weinberg’s Levels of Knowing and Existence, which he used when he covered general semantics in a graduate class on Language and Human Behavior.  Both of these works can be purchased through the IGS, as can many other fine works in this area, more than I can mention here, including Korzybski’s original works such as Science and Sanity, and Kenneth Johnson’s Outline Survey of general semantics, which I would highly recommend as a concise summary of the field.  Also of particular interest in an educational context would be two anthologies edited by Mary Morain, Teaching General Semantics: A Collection of Lesson Plans for College and Adult Classes, and Classroom Exercises in General Semantics.  In preparing these lecture notes, in addition to People in QuandariesLevels of Knowing and Existence, I have drawn upon two works that are sadly out of print, John C. Condon’s short book, Semantics and Communication, and Postman’s own work, Crazy Talk, Stupid Talk, which is based in part on People in Quandaries, advances many of the ideas, and provides an important bridge to media ecology. and



General Semantics


I.    Founded by Alfred Korzybski
A.    Korzybski was a Polish engineer who immigrated to the US, and was motivated by experiences in WWI
B.    War—communication problem
C.    When engineers get together to build a bridge, they have no problems understanding each other or getting the job done. Why shouldn’t peace be as easy to build as a bridge?
D.    Language of science was the key, and general semantics founded on idea of scientific language.  As one prominent general semanticist, Wendell Johnson, a professor of speech pathology, put it, “The language of science is the better part of the method of science.”
E.    A product of early 20th century faith in science and progress, and continued appeal to American pragmatism
F.    Highly influential in mid-20th century, proponents include S. I. Hayakawa (US Senator), Wendell Johnson (and his son Nicholas, former FCC Commissioner), Stuart Chase (economist highly influential in FDR’s New Deal), William S. Burroughs (novelist), A. E. van Vogt (sf writer, Alien), Robert Heinlein (sf writer, Stranger in a Strange Land), Frank Herbert (sf writer, Dune), Robert Anton Wilson (sf and nonfiction writer), L. Ron Hubbard (dianetics and scientology), Albert Ellis (rational emotive behavior therapy), Richard Bandler (neuro-linguistic programming), and many more
G.    More close to home, had influence on Kenneth Burke (rhetoric), Gregory Bateson (anthropology), Buckminster Fuller (design), French poststructuralists such as Derrida and Foucault, German sociologists such as Niklas Luhman, communication theorists such as Irving Lee, Elwood Murray, Tom Pace
H.    Favorable mentions from Mumford, McLuhan.  Foundational to the media ecology of Neil Postman, who edited ETC:  A Review of General Semantics, for a decade.  Pervades much of his early, collaborative work, and his important book, Crazy Talk, Stupid Talk.  An ecological approach, Korzybski described it as an “organism-as-a-whole-in-an-environment” perspective, and also makes reference to neuro-linguistic environments, neuro-semantic, and simply, semantic environments (the latter used by Wendell Johnson and Neil Postman). General semantics and the non-Aristotelian approach represents a response to the postliterate, electronic culture that has been emerging over the past century, Aristotelian thinking being a product of writing, literacy, and the alphabet.

II.    From a scientific point of view, energy is fundamental
A.    Laws of thermodynamics, Einstein’s E=MC2
B.    Energy is fundamental substance of the universe
C.    Matter is a form of energy
D.    Energy is dynamic, not static; event, not thing
E.    All forms of life based on energy
F.    Categorized into 3 classes of life based on use of energy
1.    Chemistry-Binding—plants, completely dependent on chemical processes, photosynthesis for life
2.    Space-Binding—animals, ability to move around on their own
3.    Time-Binding—humans, transmit knowledge over time, make progress. “If I can see farther, it is because I am standing on the shoulders of giants.” (Newton).  Knowledge is power, energy, most of which we inherit from previous generations
a.    progress through time-binding most apparent in science and technology—geometric advances
b.    progress in ethics, politics, economics, social and human relations, much slower—arithmetic advances
c.    given the superiority of science and technology in making progress, other sectors of society need to become more scientific in their thinking and communication
d.    scientific method, empiricism, reality-testing, use of precise language is essential

III.    Following Einstein’s non-Newtonian physics, and non-Euclidean geometry, Korzybski proposes a non-Aristotelian mode of thought and communication
A.    Not anti-Aristotle, but post-Aristotle
B.    Aristotelian Laws of Thought (rules of logic)
1.    Aristotle codified basic rules of logic, which we take for granted, seems natural to us
2.    Law of identity. A=A. A thing is what it is. A man is a man, truth is truth. A is always A; A is all A
3.    Law of excluded middle. A=B or AB, either/or. Either a man or not a man, either the truth or not the truth
4.    Law of non-contradiction. Not A=B AND AB. Not both a man and not a man, not both the truth and not the truth
5.    Laws implies permanence, static relationship, polarization, things are discrete, not process. Allows us to categorize things (no double counting to confuse inventories).
C.    Korzybki’s Non-Aristotelian Principles of Thought
1.    Principle of Non-Identity—A is not A. No identity relationships in nature. A map is not the territory it represents. The word is not the thing it represents. Whatever you say a thing is, it is not.
a.    Korzybski sat on a chair and it broke, hence a chair is not a chair.  Money is money--$100 check vs. $100 cash. Different attitudes towards money in rich, see it as symbol, not thing. Spinal tap amp “11.” Taboo words, curse words, bigot words, Lenny Bruce, body and sex words, death, disease—cancer; euphemisms
b.    Korzybski/IGS seminars.  Give out biscuits, people eat them, no problem, then show box of dog biscuits, some people feel sick, reacting to symbol or reality?  Same with chocolate/chocolate covered ants.  Cultural/symbolic prohibitions about food, kosher laws, insects (perfectly fine to eat, survival training), shell fish as insects of the sea.  Scenes from Indiana Jones and the Temple of Doom, Survivor reality TV series.
c.    Would you feel uncomfortable about drawing a mustache on a picture of your mother? Taking a pin and poking out the eyes?
2.    Principle of Non-Allness—A is not all A. By labeling, we leave out information. A writer, fine, also a criminal.
a.    A map does not represent all of a territory. Words do not say all there is to say about the things they represent. A person cannot say all there is to say about a thing. The word “is” does no mean “equals.” Johnny is bad.
b.    Danger of absolutism, universalism
3.    Principle of self-reflexiveness. An ideal map would include a map of a map, etc. It is possible to speak words about words, and words about those words, etc. It is possible to react to one’s reactions, react to those reactions, etc. Statements about statements, evaluations of evaluations. Metacommunication, recursion.
a.    Ask—describe what you are doing right now. Like mirror reflecting mirror. Infinite. Mead, consciousness and self-consciousness, imagine self as object, as others see us, and as we see them, etc. Carlyle, man is not unique because he uses tools, animals use tools. Man is unique because he uses tools to make tools. We use machines to answer and watch other machines. (VCR taping and not watching)
b.    Source of paradox.  Barber shaves every man in the village who doesn’t shave himself, who shave the barber?  To every rule there is an exception (as a rule itself).  This statement is false.
c.    Whitehead and Russell, Theory of Logical Types, class cannot be a member of itself
d.    Gödel, Incompleteness Theorem
e.    Hofstadter, Gödel, Escher, Bach; self-reflexiveness as basis of consciousness (self-consciousness)

IV.    Abstracting
A.    Key concept in general semantics
B.    Verb indicates process, as opposed to abstraction as a thing
C.    Understood in terms of levels or orders of abstracting
D.    Models:  Korzybski, Structural Differential; Hayakawa, simpler, more accessible ladder model
E.    Concept of abstracting unites perception and language/symbolic communication
F.    Levels of abstraction
1.    Event level, the reality we can never entirely know, where every phenomenon is a unique event in spacetime, no identity relationships in nature
2.    Object level, perception, organize chaotic sensory data, establish continuities, memory, recognition as basic form of identity
3.    Verbal levels, assign unique name, establishing identity over time; higher levels involve categorization
a.    Example:  Neighbor’s dog, Shirley. Shirley exists on the event level, out there in reality. 
b.    On the object level I know Shirley as a series of encounters, separate experiences, that I relate to one another.
c.    In giving these experiences a name, Shirley, I tie the experiences together to form a single identity, Shirley, as an unique individual.
d.    When I say that Shirley is a poodle, the world “poodle” here refers to a class of individuals, a category. All categories only exist in our minds. There may be a physical basis for our creation of the category, but first we must perceive and make the distinction. This is why we call such a category an abstraction, and place poodle on the second verbal level of abstracting. I may then say that a poodle is a kind of dog, and we place dog on the third level. I can go on to say that a dog is a mammal, an animal, a living thing, etc., moving higher and higher up in level of abstracting.
G.    Abstracting is a process of leaving out details, as we proceed from lower to higher levels, more details are left out
H.    Abstracting is a process of categorization, in leaving out more and more details, we put the phenomenon into categories or classes that are increasingly more general.  This is stereotyping, and one of the great successes of general semantics in the 20th century has been its contributions to educational efforts concerning stereotyping, prejudice, and scapegoating.
I.    Abstracting is a personal, subjective process where we project our evaluations onto reality. Instead of saying Shirley is a poodle, I could say that Shirley is a female.  Ask, who feels hot, who feels cold, who feels cold.  Differences in categorizing coffee as hot, pizza. Experiment:  put left hand in bucket, 35 degrees, right in 140 degrees, then both in 70 degrees. Left feels warm or hot, right feels cool or cold.
J.    The same words can have different meanings corresponding to different levels of abstracting.  This is called multiordinality.  For examples, the chair, in reference to one specific chair is on a lower level of abstracting than a chair, referring to the entire class of chairs.

V.    General Semantics Recommends
A.    Cultivate consciousness of abstracting, be aware of the process (consciousness raising).
B.    Employ an extensional orientation (inductive, gather data, check facts) rather than an intensional orientation (deductive, work off of assumptions, figure things out in your head without bothering to check).
C.    Employ a mutli-valued orientation rather than a two-valued orientation (shades of gray rather than black or white, dimmer switch rather than on or off, more than two sides to every issue).
D.    Avoid elementalism.  General semantics is a holistic, ecological approach, cautions against the way that words can separate phenomena that in reality cannot be separated, for example, space and time, mind and body, intellect and emotion.
E.    Don’t use allness words: always, never, nothing, nobody, everybody, everything, entirely, absolutely, of course, etc. (never say never).
F.    Deal in probabilities, not absolutes, use qualifying terms (probably, it seems to me, in my opinion, based on my previous experience, to the best of my recollection).
G.    Be conscious of your semantic reactions, and try to employ symbol reactions (reflective, delayed, mindful), rather than signal reactions (reflexive, immediate, kneejerk).  Traditional wisdom:  Look before you leap, count to ten, don’t count your chickens, stop, look, and listen.  Remember that there is always uncertainty, so don’t prejudge—check it out!
H.    Pavlov’s dogs. Dogs are not so bright, employ signal reactions. We need to delay reactions in order to make each reaction appropriate to the particular thing we are reacting to.
I.    Election, propaganda. React like Pavlov’s dogs to such words as abortion, welfare, taxes, inflation, war, peace.  General semantics has made significant contributions to the study, analysis, and criticism of persuasion and propaganda.
J.    Wendell Johnson, “To a mouse, cheese is cheese. That is why mouse traps are effective. To many humans being Right is Right, Wrong is Wong, Capital is Capital, and Labor is Labor. That is why propaganda is effective.”

VI.    Language and Extensional Devices
A.    Korzybski advocated the use of visual images because they are more concrete and specific than words, and favored mathematics because it is more flexible and less ambiguous than words.
B.    While general semantics typically recommends avoiding high level abstractions, Wendell Johnson cautioned against dead level abstracting, that is, staying on the same level of abstracting, rather than moving up and down the levels.
C.    Problematic nature of the verb “to be” identified by Korzybski.  When Bill Clinton said, “it depends on what the meaning of is is,” it sounded like weasel words, and was not a good thing to say under the circumstances, but actually makes sense from a general semantics perspective.  Look up “is” in the dictionary.  Actually quite complex.  Two particular problems. 
1.    One is the “is” of projection, associated with adjectives. We project all of the adjective’s qualities onto the noun it is describing. If we say that Johnny is bad, we are saying that Johnny equals bad, and thus is all bad, always bad. Influenced child psychology, don’t say you are bad; better to say, you did a bad thing; better still to say, what you did made me angry.
2.    The other problem occurs from the use of the “is” of identification. We confuse the word “is” with the word equals, and identify one noun with another. We may say that one and one is two, but do we mean the same thing when we say Joe is a criminal?  That everything there is to say about Joe is summed up by saying he is a criminal, and that everything associated with the term criminal applies to Joe?
3.    Not a problem to use “to be” as an auxiliary verb (It is raining), or to indicate existence (I am, I am that I am)
4.    Some in general semantics try to eliminate the verb “to be” entirely. E-prime (English-prime), English without the verb “to be” (several anthologies published).  Difficult, but improves writing, nothing more boring than is—It is raining vs. The rain poured down.
D.    Korzybski and others proposed the use of several devices to remind us of the non-Aristotelian laws of thought.
1.    Indexing—What is an index in a book? Use indexing—apartment numbers, street address, zip code, phone numbers, social security numbers, drivers license numbers, cataloging in Library. Algebra: X1, X2
2.    Johnny1 is not Johnny2 (people are different in different situations).  Chair1 is not chair2 (one may be cushioned, the other not, size may vary, one may break and the other not). Time1 is not time2 (things change over time).
3.    Dating—newspapers, magazines, documents, letters. Joe1980 is not Joe2010. Joe may have been a criminal then, but he could change and be a professor of law now. $22010 does not equal $21960.
4.    Et cetera—By ending every statement with etc. we are reminded that we cannot say all there is to say about anything, that there’s always more to be said, always much that we’ve left out.
5.    Hyphens: Space-time, mind-body, emotional-intellectual, to indicate that the separate terms do not exist in isolation of one another in reality
6.    Quotes: “Emotional” maladjustment, “intellectual” achievement.  In speaking, use of quotation marks gesture
7.    Plurals—Effect similar to indexing: instead of asking what is the cause of war, lower the level of abstracting by asking what are the causes of wars.  Instead of talking about falling in love as one thing, talk about fallings in loves
8.    Quantifying terms: Don’t say hot, say 95 degrees, or 70 degrees (weatherman)
9.    Qualifying terms: State exceptions, specify conditions—except, but, under conditions of, in our culture, of our time, to me, in my opinion, as I see it, from my point of view, etc. Watergate—“to the best of my recollection.”

VII.    Reification
A.    It is possible to use words that do not refer to anything in reality. The most extreme example would be nonsense words like grib and jabberwocky.  Some words refer to things that were thought to exist, but in fact do not, like phlogiston and ether.
B.    Take id, ego, supergo. They are ideas that Freud used to try to describe the way people’s minds work. But they are not things that are present in reality. Examine the brain, can’t find the id, ego, superego.
C.    High level abstractions do not refer to “things” in reality, although they seem to.  The concept of the nation is an example.  We talk about nations as if they were real things, but there is no agreement on how to define “nation” or what constitutes a nation, no objective test to determine whether some entity is a nation.  Benedict Anderson: nations are imagined communities.  Social construction.
D.    Sometimes, words that seem to refer to something real can be dangerous.  An example of this is the word “race.” One general semanticist, the anthropologist Ashley Montagu, delivered a lecture on race to the American Association of Physical Anthropologists in 1941. He used genetics as his basis for condemning the use of the term “race” among anthropologists, arguing that it concepts of race are nothing more than a product of statistical averaging.  In fact, the definition of race differs from one culture to another (in the US we traditionally defined white vs. black on an all or nothing basis of purity, whereas other cultures in the western hemisphere have at least one category of mulatto, and Brazil had 3 different races in between the extremes of black and white), and one time period to another (in the 19th century, Italians, Greeks, Jews, etc., were not considered white, and it was common to use race interchangeably with ethnicity).
E.    A similar problem occurs with the use of the word “intelligence” by social and behavioral scientists. Because the word exists, we assume that the thing it refers to exists, and we try to find it, to describe it, to measure it, to test it.  We’re not sure what it is, but we come up with a test for it anyway.  And in the end we mistake the score on an intelligence test for intelligence itself. The score is real, intelligence is not. The score is merely a measure of an individual’s ability to take the test. Stephen Jay Gould, The Mismeasure of Man, has argued that intelligence testing has been used (perhaps unintentionally) to “prove” that white Americans are superior, in intelligence, to non-whites and immigrants.  People institutionalized for scoring a few points below the minimum legal requirement on intelligence tests, even though they are capable of taking care of themselves.  Howard Gardner, theory of multiple intelligences, extensional device of using plurals.
F.    Modern maps, north as up, top, arbitrary, ideological, vs. ancient orientation
G.    As we go higher up in levels of abstracting, we reach the point where there are words about words, and not things. But because the word exists, we believe that the thing must exist, we must thingify the word. This is called reification, to make real. When we try to find and measure abstractions like intelligence, patriotism, love, we are guilty of reification.

VIII.    Idealization and Operationalism
A.    Wendell Johnson believed that mistaking high-level abstractions for things often leads to the IFD disease. The IFD disease refers to the following process: Idealization. Frustration. Demoralization.
B.    For example, most people have the same goals: success, happiness, love, peace, fulfillment. Counselors and therapists are constantly finding patients who have been demoralized in their search for these goals, because they have idealized the terms, and therefore have no concrete indicators for having reached the goals.  Love is often idealized, so much so that no one seems to be good enough.
C.    Success is a good example.  Ask just about anyone who is a part of American culture if he or she wants to be a success, and he or she will say yes (indeed it’s an act of courage to say otherwise).  But how do you know when you are a success?  Therapists say that it’s not unusual to find people coming to them who are making a good living, their children are healthy, they have a summer home, their wives or husbands are agreeable, loving people, and yet, somehow there is something missing, something else, but they don’t know what it looks like or where it is. The problem is that they have idealized the word success. In idealizing success, you have placed it forever out of reach. This leads to frustration, and eventually to demoralization.  Take the even more basic example of rich.  Many people would like to be rich, but few think about how to determine if someone is rich or not.  What does it take to be rich? 
D.    The answer to this problem comes from science. Science teaches us that all terms must have operational definitions. Operational definitions are concrete definitions.  An experiment that calls for water specifies exactly what type of water is to be used, from what source (specific lake, river, rain), what type (distilled, demineralized, condensed steam), etc.  So, for example, if you say that being rich is having a million dollars in the bank, then your goal becomes measurable, and attainable.  When you reach the goal, you can feel a sense of satisfaction, pat yourself on the back.  You still can set new goals, of course, you don’t have to stop there. 
E.    If you say you want to be a rock star, operationalize it.  How would you define it.  Playing a concert for a certain number of people?  Releasing a recording?  A certain level of sales?  Signed by a label or going on tour?  Once you set a concrete goal, you can also outline the steps it would take to get there, the operations required to reach the goal.  This is how career counselors help people, by getting them to set goals, decide on whether they are realistic and obtainable (if not, you need to revise your goals), and then figure out the steps you need to get there.  This way, you have a plan to follow, and can also determine if you are not going to meet your goal and it’s best to move on.

IX.    Facts
A.    A fact is not so much a truth as it is a statement about truth.  Facts are commonly thought to be true, but actually are statements that can be proven true of false.
B.    A fact, is also know as a statement of fact, as a statement of description, and as a proposition (as in, this is proposed to be true).
C.     Scientific language uses propositions.  Simple statements of description are relatively low level abstracting, and can be proven true or false based on observation. “It is raining outside right now,” is a proposition. We can look out of the window or go outside and determine its validity.
D.    Generalizations represent a higher level of abstracting, for example, whenever dark clouds gather, rain follows.  We can test the generalization and see whether it holds true in this particular instance.  If it doesn’t, we have proven it false.  If it does, however, we have not proven it true, because we cannot text for every possible instance, past, present, and future.  Karl Popper.  All swans are white.
E.    No statement, no idea, no concept, no theory can be called scientific if it does not allow the possibility of being proven false.
F.    “He has $100,000 is a proposition, if we can gain access to the appropriate information, and prove it true or false. “He is rich,” is not a proposition, unless we give the word “rich” an operational definition that allows for testing.
G.    Not all statements are propositions.  Most are not.  Most statements cannot be proven true or false, or falsified.
H.    Truth in Advertising laws only go so far.  If the ad says the price is $100, and they charge you $200, that’s a false claim.  If the ad says the product does the job better than its competitor, and there is an objective test to determine whether that is so, the ad is factual, propositional.  If the ad says the product is cheaper and better, works faster, etc., without making a comparison, there is no statement that can be tested, therefore no false claim.  How can you prove true or false a statement like, “You’re in good hands with Allstate”; “Drivers wanted”; “I love New York”; or “Got milk?”
I.    Some statements are simply definitions, axioms, and in this sense tautolgies (it is what it is because I say it is).  Let x=3.  A dog is a four-legged mammal that barks.  A sentence is a complete thought.  You cannot prove a definition true or false, you can only accept or reject it.
J.    Some statements are judgments or opinions, expressing values and emotions.  He is evil is a value judgment, as opposed to saying he has been arrested and convicted of a crime.  Ethics and aesthetics are based on judgments.  You cannot prove that anything or anyone is bad or good, beautiful or ugly, etc.
K.    Some statements are inferences, which are assumptions (and what happens when you assume?).  They resemble propositions or facts, in that they are statements that could be proven true or false under the right conditions.  But they are deductions or leaps made without access to the need evidence.  I say it’s raining outside right now, there’s a window, so you look out and see that it is, that’s a proposition.  I say it’s raining outside right now, there’s no window, you know it was raining when you came in, so you figure it must still be raining, that’s an inference.
L.    Distinguishing Between Facts and Inferences
1.    Based on the information you have available to you right now, without further investigation, is the statement “I am standing here” a fact or an inference? Is the statement “I am wearing underwear” a fact or inference?
2.    In detective stories the hero finds clues and solves the crime by making inferences based on the clues.  Using inferences is a sign of intelligence, often tested for in grade school standardized tests for language/reading (which is the best title for this story?).  But distinguishing between facts and inferences is a more advanced form of intelligence, appears on IQ tests and LSATs.
3.    Exercise:  Fact/Inference Test
4.    Inferences are used in persuasion and propaganda.  Misleading association—actual association—implied association. Presidential candidate Adlai Stevenson worked in state department at the same time as Alger Hiss, who has been convicted of conspiring for communism. US corporation profits doubled in 1968, US casualties in Vietnam doubled in 1968.
5.    Inferences used in politics.  News leaks, background briefings, pseudo-events.  The president said, X.  This reporter was also told Y.
6.    News reporting, especially based on government or political sources, full of ambiguous language, suggesting inferences, but with deniability.
7.    Columnist has someone else do interview for him, write it in passive voice so he never says he asked the questions.
8.    General semantics, understanding of propositions and inferences, scientific approach, very useful for journalists, problem of objectivity similar to that in science, reporting the facts
9.    Images make no statements (Susanne Langer, Philosophy in a New Key), are not propositional or discursive, cannot be proven true or false.  The can be used as evidence to support a claim, but make no claim of their own.  As opposed to me saying, it is raining outside right now, if I show you a picture taken outside of rain, it makes no statement, it is what it is, a concrete image of a moment in time.  Only if I add the words, this is what it’s doing outside right now do we have a proposition.  A picture of a bombed out building can be accompanied by a caption that says it was a military target, or a civilian residence.  The same picture can be used to make contradictory statements, used by opposite sides.  That’s why we need to read the caption on an illustration, check the title card of a painting in an art museum.  The words tell us what we’re seeing.  Pictures are not worth a 1,000 words, not translatable, apples and oranges.  Case of Rodney King, first trial, prosecution let the video recording of the beating stand on its own, major mistake, as defense deconstructed the images.
10.    Ability to recognize the difference between facts and inference important in the legal sector, reading contracts, understand what they say, and what they don’t say
11.    Robert Heinlein in the science fiction novel Stranger In a Strange Land introduces the concept of the fair witness. A fair witness is a person who is specially trained to only report facts and make no inferences. For example, someone would ask a fair witness what color is that house. The fair witness would answer,” it is white on this side.”
12.    A colleague of mine served on a jury once, and got into an argument with her fellow jurors over a piece of evidence. The jurors all believe that the suspect was wearing a green army jacket. This evidence was based on a black and white photograph. The jurors could not distinguish between fact and inference, and this distinction is especially important in a trial.
13.    General semantics does not say “Never make an inference.” Inferences are a necessary part of our lives. What General Semantics does say is know the difference between an inference and a fact, and be aware of the fact that when you make an inference or an assumption, you may be making a mistake.

X.    Conclusion
A.    The major assumption of general semantics is that we have a constant need for a constant evaluation and constant re-evaluation because time is constantly flowing and change is constantly occurring.
B.    How we label things determines how we react to them. Power over how we label things is power over our reactions.
C.    The ability to change definitions, to not be tied to one definition, and also to be able to change levels of abstractions, is called reframing, and is an essential skill.
D.    Sanity, individually and most importantly collectively requires that we create and utilize maps that are as similar as possible to the environment that they represent.  Our maps may be subjective, and a product of social construction (intersubjectivity), but all maps are not alike, not equally valid or useful.  Some maps will leave us lost, others will allow us to arrive at our destination safe and sound.